| Peer-Reviewed

The Factorial Structure of Self-Regulation from Late Childhood to Adolescence: A Gender Perspective

Received: 31 January 2023    Accepted: 13 March 2023    Published: 28 March 2023
Views:       Downloads:
Abstract

The present study aimed to examine the factorial structure of self-regulation across three different groups (i.e. late childhood, early adolescence, and middle, adolescence) within male and female samples. Two theoretical models were tested. The first is the unidimensional model which assumes that self-regulation is a domain-general ability without clear differentiation between its components (cognitive, emotional, and behavioral). The second model implies that self-regulation is conceptualized as a multidimensional construct that’s composed of cognitive emotional, and behavioral components. Objective: The current study examined the validity of the unidimensional model and the multidimensional model of self-regulation across the 3 age groups separated by gender. In addition, the study sought to identify gender differences in the factorial structure of self-regulation in children and adolescents. Materials and methods: The participants comprised 584 participants aged 8 to 19 years. They were enrolled in Elementary, middle and high schools in Egypt. The participants completed a self-regulation scale consisting of three subscales designed to evaluate emotional, cognitive, and behavioral self-regulation. Results: The results favored the multidimensional model across all different age groups. The difference between the two models was statistically significant favoring the multidimensional model over the one-factor solution. The results also revealed within genders t differences in the developmental course of self-regulation.

Published in American Journal of Applied Psychology (Volume 12, Issue 1)
DOI 10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13
Page(s) 16-27
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Structure of Self-Regulation, Gender Differences, Late Adulthood, Adolescence

References
[1] Anderson, V., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Catroppa, C. (2001a). Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20 (1), 385–406.
[2] Baddeley, A. D. (2000). Short-term and working memory. The Oxford handbook of memory, 4, 77-92.
[3] Barkley, R. A. (1997). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: toward a more comprehensive theory. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics.
[4] Bashir, H., & Bashir, L. (2016). Gender difference on self -regulation among adolescents. Indian Journal of Applied Research, 6 (3), 694-695.
[5] Berger, A., Kaufman, O., Liven, U., & Henrik, A. (2007). Multidisciplinary perspectives on attention and the development of self-regulation. Progress in neurobiology, 82 (5), 256-286.
[6] Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81 (6), 1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/J.1467-8624.2010.01499.x.
[7] Bjork Lund, D. F., and Kipp, K. (1996). Parental investment theory and gender differences in the evolution of inhibition mechanisms. Psychol. Bull. 120, 163–188. doi: DOI: 1037/0033-2909.120.2.163.
[8] Blair, C., Calkins, S., & Kopp, L. (2010). Self-regulation as the interface of emotional and cognitive development: Implications for education and academic achievement.
[9] Bridges, L. J., Denham, S. A., & Ganiban, J. M. (2004). Definitional Issues in Emotion Regulation Research. Child Development, 75 (2), 340–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00675.x.
[10] Calkins, S. D. (2010). Commentary: Conceptual and methodological challenges to the study of emotion regulation. Journal of psychopathology and Assessment, 32 (1), 92– 95.
[11] Carlson, S. M., Zelazo, P. D., & Faja, S. (2013). Executive function. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology, Body and mind (Vol. 1, pp. 706– 743). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
[12] Carducci, Bernardo J. (2009). "Basic Processes of Mitchel’s Cognitive-Affective Perspective: Delay of Gratification and Conditions of Behavioral Consistency". The Psychology of Personality: Viewpoints, Research, and Applications. John Wiley and Sons. pp. 443–448.
[13] Carroll, M. E., Kohl, E. A., Johnson, K. M., and Lanasa, R. M. (2013). The increase choice for saccharin during PCP withdrawal in female monkeys: influence of menstrual cycle phase. Psychopharmacology 227, 413–424.
[14] Cauchy, M. O., Mills, B., Brinkley, D., & Owen, M. T. (2018). Behavioral self‐regulation, early academic achievement, and the effectiveness of urban schools for low‐income ethnic minority children. American journal of community psychology, 61 (3-4), 372-385.
[15] Ceresnik, (2013). Self-regulation of Adolescents. Gender specifications. Procedia-social and Behavioral Sciences, 106, 2365-2374.
[16] Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual review of psychology, 52, 337–367. doi: 10.1146/annual rev.psych.52.1.337.
[17] Cowan N, Saults JS, Elliot EM. The search for what is fundamental in the development of working memory. Adv. Child Dev. Behavior. 2002; 29: 1–49.
[18] Demetriou, A. (2000). Organization and Development of Self-Understanding and Self-Regulation. Handbook of Self-Regulation, 209–251. doi: 10.1016/b978-012109890-2/50036-6.
[19] Diamond, A. (2013). Self-regulation and executive functions. Paper presented at the SRCD/CASTL Measures Meeting, Charlottesville, VA.
[20] Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466–503). London, England: Oxford University Press.
[21] Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Self-discipline gives girls the edge: Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (1), 198–208. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198
[22] East, Inge-Marie; Zayas, Vivian; Mitchel, Walter; Shoda, Yuichi; Ayduk, Ozlem; Dadlani, Mamta B.; Davidson, Matthew C.; Aber, J. Lawrence; Casey, B. J. (2006). "Predicting Cognitive Control from Preschool to Late Adolescence and Young adulthood". Psychological Science. 17 (6): 478–84.
[23] Edossa, A. K., Schroeders, U., Weinert, S., & Artelt, C. (2017). The development of emotional and behavioral self-regulation and their effects on academic achievement in childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42 (2), 192–202. doi: 10.1177/0165025416687412.
[24] Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Bernzweig, J., Karbon, M., Poulin, R., &. Hanish, L. (1993). The relations of emotionality and regulation to preschoolers’ social skills and sociometric status. Child Development, 64, 1418–1.
[25] Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B. C. Maskzk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 66, 1360–1384.
[26] Elizabeth P. Shulman, K. Paige Harden, Jason M. Chein & Laurence Steinberg (2015) Gender Differences in the Developmental Trajectories of Impulse Control and Sensation Seeking from Early Adolescence to Early Adulthood, Journal of Youth and adolescence volume 44, pages 1–17.
[27] Faja, S., & Dawson, G. (2013). Reduced delay of gratification and effortful control among young children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 19 (1), 91–01. doi: 10.1177/1362361313512424.
[28] Fung, W. K., Chung, K. K. H., & Cheng, R. W. Y. (2019). Gender differences in social mastery motivation and its relationships to vocabulary knowledge, behavioral self-regulation, and socioemotional skills. Early Education and Development, 30 (2), 280-293.
[29] Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40 (2), 177–190. doi: 10.1037/0012- 1649.40.2.177.
[30] Gestsdottir, S., von Suchodoletz, A., Wanless, S. B., Hubert, B., Gruinard, P., Birgisdottir, F., & McClelland, M. (2014). Early behavioral self-regulation, academic achievement, and gender: Longitudinal findings from France, Germany, and Iceland. Applied Developmental Science, 18 (2), 90-109.
[31] Gestsdottir, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2008). Positive development in adolescence: The development and role of intentional self-regulation. Human Development, 51, 202–224.
[32] Gross, J. J., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2011). Emotion Generation and Emotion Regulation: One or Two Depends on Your Point of View. Emotion Review, 3 (1), 8–16. doi: 10.1177/1754073910380974.
[33] Gross, J. and Thompson R. Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations. In: J. J. Gross (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. (ed.); New York: The Guilford Press; pp. 324.
[34] Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion regulation: Past, present, and future. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 551–573.
[35] Hartley, B. L., & Sutton, R. M. (2013). A stereotype threat account of boys' academic underachievement. Child development, 84 (5), 1716-1733.
[36] Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., & Lofaro, N. (2016). The Times They Are a-Changing … or Are They Not? A Comparison of Gender Stereotypes, 1983–2014. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40 (3), 353–363. Doi: 10.1177/0361684316634081.
[37] Hammer, D., Melhuish, E., & Howard, S. (2015). The nature and importance of self-regulation in early childhood: Factor structure and predictive validity (p. 41). Abstract presented at the 17th European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Braga, Portugal.
[38] Haslam D., Mejia A, Dana T., Betancourt T. (2019). Self- Regulation in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: Challenges and Future Directions, Clinical Child and Family Psychology, Review (2019) 22: 104–117.
[39] Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 363–390. doi: 10.1146/annualrev.psych.121208.131616.
[40] Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (4), 962–977. doi: 10.1037/a0012705.
[41] Hosseini-Kamkar, N., & Morton, J. B. (2014). Gender differences in self-regulation: an evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in neuroscience, 8, 233.
[42] Kalpidou, M. D., Power, T. G., Cherry, K. E., & Gottfried, N. W. (2004). Regulation of emotion and behavior among 3- and 5-year-olds. The Journal of General Psychology, 131, 159–178. doi: 10.3200/GENP.131.2.159-180.
[43] Khaighobadi, F., and Stevens, J. (2013). Does fertility status influence impulsivity and risk taking in human females? Adaptive influences on intertemporal choice and risky decision making. Evolutionary. Psychol. 11, 700–717.
[44] Kurman, J. (2001). Self-regulation strategies in achievement settings: Culture and gender differences. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (4), 491-503.
[45] Levesque, R. J. R. (2018). Effortful Control. In: Levesque, R. J. R. (Eds) Encyclopedia of Adolescence. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-33228-4_534
[46] Jurado B. M., Monica R. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. 17 (3): 213-33.
[47] Labuhn, A. S., Zimmerman, B. J., & Hassel horn, M. (2010). Enhancing students’ self-regulation and school the influence of feedback and self-evaluative standards. Metacognition and learning, 5 (2), 173-194.
[48] Larson, R. W. (2011). Adolescents’ conscious processes of developing regulation: Learning to appraise challenges. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2011 (133), 87–97.
[49] Lee, K., Bull, R., & Ho, R. M. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child development, 84 (6), 1933-1953.
[50] Lerner, R. M., & Steinberg, L. D. (2009). The scientific study of adolescent development: Historical and contemporary perspectives. In R. M. Lerner & L. D. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 3–15). Hoboken, N. J: Wiley.
[51] Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2010). Gender differences in the adolescent brain. Brain and Cognition, 72, 46-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008
[52] Matthews, J. S., Marulis, L. M., & Williford, A. P. (2014). Gender processes in school functioning and the mediating role of cognitive self-regulation. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35 (3), 128-137.
[53] Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101 (3), 689– 704.
[54] McClelland, M., Geldhof, J., Morrison, F., Gestsdóttir, S., Cameron, C., Bowers, E., Grammer, J. (2017). Self-Regulation. Handbook of Life Course Health Development, 275– 298. Doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-47143-3_12.
[55] McClelland, M. M., John Geldof, G., Cameron, C. E., & Wanless, S. B. (2015). Development and self-regulation. Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, 1-43.
[56] McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Duncan, R., Bowles, R. P., A cock, A. C., Miao, A., & Pratt, M. E. (2014). Predictors of Earl behavioral regulation y Growth in Academic Achievement: The Head-Toes Knees-Shoulders Task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 599616. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00599
[57] McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2011). Self-Regulation in Early Childhood: Improving Conceptual Clarity and Developing Ecologically Valid Measures. Child Development Perspectives, 6 (2), 136–142. doi: 10.1111/j.1750- 606.2011.00191.x.
[58] McClelland, M. M., Ponitz, C. C., Messersmith, E. E., & Tominey, S. (2010). Self-Regulation. The Handbook of Lifespan Development. Doi: 10.1002/9780470880166.hlsd001015.
[59] Mischel, W., Ayduk, O., Berman, M. G., Casey, B. J., Gotlib, I. H., Jonides, Jshoda, Y. (2011). “Willpower” over the life span: Decomposing self-regulation. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 252–256.
[60] Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.
[61] Mischel, W., & Underwood, B. (1974). Instrumental ideation in the delay of gratification. Child development, 10831088.
[62] Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current directions in psychological science, 21 (1), 8-14.
[63] Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41 (1), 49-100.
[64] Moilanen, k. l., & DeLong, k. l. (2017). Self-regulation. In R. J. Levesque (ed.), encyclopedia of adolescence. 2nd edition. New York, NY: Springer.
[65] Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The adolescent self-regulatory inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36 (6), 835–848.
[66] Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 361–383. Doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675.
[67] Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior. In: R. J. Davidson., G. E. Schwartz, & D. E. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and Self-Regulation (pp. 1-14). New York: Plenum Press.
[68] Novak, S. P., & Clayton, R. R. (2001). The influence of school environment and self-regulation on transitions between stages of cigarette smoking: A multilevel analysis. Health Psychology, 20 (3), 196–207. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.20.3.196.
[69] Pajares F. & VALIANTE, G. (2002). (2002). Students Self Efficacy in Their Self-Regulated Learning Strategies. PSYCHOLOGIA -An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 45 (4), 211–221. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2002.211.
[70] Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12 (3), 427–441.
[71] Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention, self-regulation, and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 353 (1377), 1915–1927.
[72] Pine, K. J., and Fletcher, B. (2011). Women's spending behavior is menstrual-cycle sensitive, with Personality and Individual Differences. 50, 74–78.
[73] Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. The journal of genetic psychology, 166 (1), 54-76.
[74] Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Pianta, R. C. (2000). An Ecological Perspective on the Transition to Kindergarten. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21 (5), 491–511. Doi: 10.1016/s0193-3973(00)00051-4.
[75] Roebers, C. M. (2017). Executive function and metacognition: Towards a unifying framework of cognitive self-regulation. Developmental Review, 45, 31-51.
[76] Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development. 6th ed. NJ, John Wiley & Sons Inc.: Hoboken; 2006: 99-166.
[77] Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Genes and experience in the development of executive attention and effortful control. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 109, 101–108. doi: 10.1002/cd.142.
[78] Rueda MR, Cómbita LM. Best practices in the development of effortful control in early childhood. Tremblay RE, topic Ed. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2012: 1-7. http://www.child-ncyclopedia.com / Accessed [3/1/23].
[79] Sheese, B. E., Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., White, L. K., & Fraundorf, S. H. (2008). Executive attention and self-regulation in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 31 (3), 501–510. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.02.001.
[80] Shields, A. M., Cicchetti, D., & Ryan, R. M. (1994). The development of emotional and behavioral self-regulation and social competence among maltreated school-age children. Development and Psychopathology, 6 (01), 57– 75. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400005885.
[81] Silverman, I. W. (2003). Gender differences in delay of gratification: A meta-analysis. Gender roles, 49 (9), 451-463.
[82] Smith, C., Sierra, Y., Oppler, S., and Boettiger, C. (2014). Ovarian cycle effects on immediate reward selection bias in humans: a role for estradiol. J. Neuroscience. 34, 5468–5476.
[83] Sokol, B., Müller, U., Carpendale, J. I. M, Young, A. R., & Iarocci, G. (2010). Self-and social-regulation: Exploring the relations between social interaction, social cognition, and the development of executive functions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
[84] Storksen, I., Ellingsen, I. T., Wanless, S. B., & McClelland, M. M. (2015). The influence of parental socioeconomic background and gender on self-regulation among 5-year-old children in Norway. Early Education and Development, 26 (5-6), 663-684.
[85] Sullivan, T. N., Helms, S. W., Kliewer, W., & Goodman, K. L. (2010). Associations between Sadness and Anger Regulation Coping, Emotional Expression, and Physical and Relational Aggression among Urban Adolescents. Social Development, 19 (1), 30–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00531.x.
[86] Tetering, M. V., Laan, A. V. D., Kogel, C. D., Groot, R. D., & Jolles, J. (2020). Gender differences in self-regulation in early, middle and late adolescence: A large-scale cross-sectional study. PLoS One, 15 (1), e0227607.
[87] Vink, M., Gladwin, T. E., Geeraerts, S., Pas, P., Bos, D., Hofstee, M& Vollebergh, W. (2020). Towards an integrated account of the development of self-regulation from a neurocognitive perspective: A framework for current and future longitudinal multi-modal investigations. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, 100829.
[88] Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S. H., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., & Sung, M. (2013). Gender differences in behavioral regulation in four societies: The United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28 (3), 621-633.
[89] Weis, M., Heikamp, T., and Trommsdorff, G. (2013). Gender differences in school achievement: the role of self-regulation. Front. Psychol. 4: 42. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00442.
[90] Yamamoto, N., & Imai-Matsumura, K. (2019). Gender differences in executive function and behavioral self-regulation in 5 years old kindergarteners from East Japan. Early Child Development and Care, 189 (1), 56-67.
[91] Wang Y. Xin C. (2022). Early childhood relational contexts contribute to executive function in first grade. Journal of Psychology, 101367.
[92] Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7 (2), 131–149.
[93] Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 55-68.
[94] Zimmermann, L. K., & Stansbury, K. (2003). The influence of temperamental reactivity and situational context on the emotion-regulatory abilities of 3-year-old children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164 (4), 389–409.
[95] Zimmermann P., Alexandra I., (2014). Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38 (2) 182–194.
[96] Zimmermann P., Thompson R. A. (2014). New directions in developmental emotion regulation research across the life span Introduction to the special section. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38 (2): 139-141.
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Asmaa Mohamed Reyad, Amany Ahmed Yehia, Fadya Zaky Elwan. (2023). The Factorial Structure of Self-Regulation from Late Childhood to Adolescence: A Gender Perspective. American Journal of Applied Psychology, 12(1), 16-27. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Asmaa Mohamed Reyad; Amany Ahmed Yehia; Fadya Zaky Elwan. The Factorial Structure of Self-Regulation from Late Childhood to Adolescence: A Gender Perspective. Am. J. Appl. Psychol. 2023, 12(1), 16-27. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Asmaa Mohamed Reyad, Amany Ahmed Yehia, Fadya Zaky Elwan. The Factorial Structure of Self-Regulation from Late Childhood to Adolescence: A Gender Perspective. Am J Appl Psychol. 2023;12(1):16-27. doi: 10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13,
      author = {Asmaa Mohamed Reyad and Amany Ahmed Yehia and Fadya Zaky Elwan},
      title = {The Factorial Structure of Self-Regulation from Late Childhood to Adolescence: A Gender Perspective},
      journal = {American Journal of Applied Psychology},
      volume = {12},
      number = {1},
      pages = {16-27},
      doi = {10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ajap.20231201.13},
      abstract = {The present study aimed to examine the factorial structure of self-regulation across three different groups (i.e. late childhood, early adolescence, and middle, adolescence) within male and female samples. Two theoretical models were tested. The first is the unidimensional model which assumes that self-regulation is a domain-general ability without clear differentiation between its components (cognitive, emotional, and behavioral). The second model implies that self-regulation is conceptualized as a multidimensional construct that’s composed of cognitive emotional, and behavioral components. Objective: The current study examined the validity of the unidimensional model and the multidimensional model of self-regulation across the 3 age groups separated by gender. In addition, the study sought to identify gender differences in the factorial structure of self-regulation in children and adolescents. Materials and methods: The participants comprised 584 participants aged 8 to 19 years. They were enrolled in Elementary, middle and high schools in Egypt. The participants completed a self-regulation scale consisting of three subscales designed to evaluate emotional, cognitive, and behavioral self-regulation. Results: The results favored the multidimensional model across all different age groups. The difference between the two models was statistically significant favoring the multidimensional model over the one-factor solution. The results also revealed within genders t differences in the developmental course of self-regulation.},
     year = {2023}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - The Factorial Structure of Self-Regulation from Late Childhood to Adolescence: A Gender Perspective
    AU  - Asmaa Mohamed Reyad
    AU  - Amany Ahmed Yehia
    AU  - Fadya Zaky Elwan
    Y1  - 2023/03/28
    PY  - 2023
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13
    DO  - 10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13
    T2  - American Journal of Applied Psychology
    JF  - American Journal of Applied Psychology
    JO  - American Journal of Applied Psychology
    SP  - 16
    EP  - 27
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2328-5672
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20231201.13
    AB  - The present study aimed to examine the factorial structure of self-regulation across three different groups (i.e. late childhood, early adolescence, and middle, adolescence) within male and female samples. Two theoretical models were tested. The first is the unidimensional model which assumes that self-regulation is a domain-general ability without clear differentiation between its components (cognitive, emotional, and behavioral). The second model implies that self-regulation is conceptualized as a multidimensional construct that’s composed of cognitive emotional, and behavioral components. Objective: The current study examined the validity of the unidimensional model and the multidimensional model of self-regulation across the 3 age groups separated by gender. In addition, the study sought to identify gender differences in the factorial structure of self-regulation in children and adolescents. Materials and methods: The participants comprised 584 participants aged 8 to 19 years. They were enrolled in Elementary, middle and high schools in Egypt. The participants completed a self-regulation scale consisting of three subscales designed to evaluate emotional, cognitive, and behavioral self-regulation. Results: The results favored the multidimensional model across all different age groups. The difference between the two models was statistically significant favoring the multidimensional model over the one-factor solution. The results also revealed within genders t differences in the developmental course of self-regulation.
    VL  - 12
    IS  - 1
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Cairo University, Cairo, Egypt

  • Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Cairo University, Cairo, Egypt

  • Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Cairo University, Cairo, Egypt

  • Sections